Skip to main content

Test–retest reliability of multidimensional dyspnea profile recall ratings in the emergency department: a prospective, longitudinal study

Abstract

Background

Dyspnea is among the most common reasons for emergency department (ED) visits by patients with cardiopulmonary disease who are commonly asked to recall the symptoms that prompted them to come to the ED. The reliability of recalled dyspnea has not been systematically investigated in ED patients.

Methods

Patients with chronic or acute cardiopulmonary conditions who came to the ED with dyspnea (N = 154) completed the Multidimensional Dyspnea Profile (MDP) several times during the visit and in a follow-up visit 4 to 6 weeks later (n = 68). The MDP has 12 items with numerical ratings of intensity, unpleasantness, sensory qualities, and emotions associated with how breathing felt when participants decided to come to the ED (recall MDP) or at the time of administration (“now” MDP). The recall MDP was administered twice in the ED and once during the follow-up visit. Principal components analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation was used to assess domain structure of the recall MDP. Internal consistency reliability was assessed with Cronbach’s alpha. Test–retest reliability was assessed with intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) for absolute agreement for individual items and domains.

Results

PCA of the recall MDP was consistent with two domains (Immediate Perception, 7 items, Cronbach’s alpha = .89 to .94; Emotional Response, 5 items; Cronbach’s alpha = .81 to .85). Test–retest ICCs for the recall MDP during the ED visit ranged from .70 to .87 for individual items and were .93 and .94 for the Immediate Perception and Emotional Response domains. ICCs were much lower for the interval between the ED visit and follow-up, both for individual items (.28 to .66) and for the Immediate Perception and Emotional Response domains (.72 and .78, respectively).

Conclusions

During an ED visit, recall MDP ratings of dyspnea at the time participants decided to seek care in the ED are reliable and sufficiently stable, both for individual items and the two domains, that a time lag between arrival and questionnaire administration does not critically affect recall of perceptual and emotional characteristics immediately prior to the visit. However, test–retest reliability of recall over a 4- to 6-week interval is poor for individual items and significantly attenuated for the two domains.

Peer Review reports

Background

Recall of symptoms leading to an emergency department (ED) visit is a mainstay of clinical history-taking. Patients are commonly asked about symptoms or events prior to arrival that prompted the visit, but little is known about the reliability of recall self-reports, especially in relation to elapsed time. Accurate assessment of past symptoms is a key component of clinical decision making, including the choice of initial therapies, as well as consultation and hospitalization decisions. The reliability of symptom recall is also a potential concern in clinical research on symptoms in acute care settings because of time constraints on when patients can be approached, adequately informed of study purposes, and consented as research participants.

Several studies have examined test–retest reliability of self-reports of various symptoms during an emergency visit [14], although none involved recalling symptoms prior to the visit. For example, 100-mm visual analog scale pain ratings taken 1 minute apart were highly consistent (intraclass correlation coefficient [ICC] = .99) in a study of acute abdominal pain in ED patients [1]. Similarly, a study of acute pain in a pediatric ED showed high consistency in pain reports 1 to 3 hours apart using a 0-to-10 numerical rating scale in older children (≥ 8 years of age). The mean difference in pain ratings among those reporting no change was 0.2 scale points (95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.0, 0.4) [2]. By contrast, poor agreement was reported for repeated measures of descriptors of acute dizziness (e.g., spinning, unsteady, about to faint) in adult ED patients [3]. In another study, adult ED patients’ ratings of headache severity showed, at best, only moderate within-subjects agreement (κ = .51) and substantial within-subjects discordance (23%–38% of subjects) in responses to two semantically similar questions about present headache severity in relation to past history [4].

Symptom recall has been called “deceptively…complex” [5]. The reliability and validity of recall ratings depend on how patients are instructed, how many and which dimensions or characteristics they are asked to rate, and whether they are asked to recall a specific event, a particular interval (e.g., over the last 24 hours, week, or month) or some undefined usual state in relation to present discomfort [6].

Dyspnea is defined as “a subjective experience of breathing discomfort that consists of qualitatively distinct sensations that vary in intensity,” that involves “interactions among multiple physiological, psychological, social, and environmental factors, and may induce secondary physiological and behavioral responses” [7, 8]. As a subjective experience, dyspnea is typically measured using various rating scales or questionnaires, many of which assess the impact of dyspnea on activity, functional status, or quality of life, rather than what breathing feels like [8]. Only a few of these instruments have been validated in ED patients [9, 10].

In several studies conducted in EDs, dyspnea has been measured with a single-item rating such as a visual analog scale, numerical rating scale, or Borg scale [1015]. A potential limitation of single-item scales is that unless instructions are clear about what aspect of the symptom to rate (e.g., how intense it is or how distressed one is by it) and consistently given, the symptom dimension being measured is potentially ambiguous [16]. In addition, the internal consistency reliability of single items is indeterminate (e.g., Cronbach’s coefficient alpha cannot be estimated for a single item) [17, 18]. Although test–retest reliability of single items can be assessed, this can be challenging in the ED if what is being measured changes in response to acute treatment.

We are aware of only one study of dyspnea in an ED that assessed the test–retest reliability of recall ratings of dyspnea prior to a visit [19]. The median correlation for numeric ratings of seven dyspnea descriptors was .95, and the largest within-subjects difference for any descriptor was less than 1 point on a 0-to-10 numeric scale. However, that assessment was conducted with just a small subset (~10%) of the study sample, and conventional correlation coefficients are suboptimal for assessing test–retest reliability [18, 2022]. Results of that study [19] also suggested several potentially distinguishable dimensions of sensory quality in ED patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), three of which (Smothering/Air hunger, Work/Effort, and Tightness) were confirmed in a subsequent study of hospital patients admitted for heart failure [23]. Only a few other studies have attempted to assess sensory qualities of dyspnea during ED visits [24, 25]. The results of these studies suggest that multiple sensory quality dimensions of dyspnea may be common to patients of various diagnoses who come to an ED because of dyspnea. Although the clinical relevance of multiple dimensions of dyspnea in the acute care setting is not clearly established, in a study of ED patients with asthma, it was found that perceptions of increased work and effort in breathing persisted even after the sensation of tightness was relieved with albuterol [24].

The purpose of the present study was to assess the reliability of recall ratings of dyspnea in ED patients with acute or chronic pulmonary or cardiac disease. Specifically, we administered the Multidimensional Dyspnea Profile (MDP) [2628] to obtain serial real-time and recall ratings during an ED visit and in an outpatient follow-up visit 4 to 6 weeks later. Results pertaining to the real-time ratings and overall psychometric performance of the MDP are being published separately [28].

Methods

Design, setting, and participants

The study had a prospective, longitudinal correlational design with repeated measures. The study was conducted in three urban EDs in the southwestern United States: at an academic health center, a Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) medical center, and a private, not-for-profit community hospital. The study was approved by the Human Research Review Committee of the University of New Mexico Health Sciences Center and the Raymond G. Murphy VA Medical Center Research and Development Committee, Albuquerque, NM. Signed, informed consent and Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act (HIPAA) authorization were obtained from all participants. All recruitment and data collection were the responsibility of study personnel, who were credentialed by each facility in accordance with its research policies.

Patients who presented to the ED with breathing complaints due to acute or chronic pulmonary or cardiac conditions were potentially eligible. Exclusion criteria were: treatment for an acute coronary syndrome or advanced or metastatic cancer; absence of dyspnea at presentation; inability to speak or understand English; or previous participation in the study. Of 526 potentially eligible patients, 94 were discharged before recruitment could be completed. Of the remaining 432 patients, 182 (42%) agreed to participate.

Measures

The MDP [26, 28] was developed by an interdisciplinary team with expertise in respiratory physiology and psychophysics, pulmonary and critical care medicine, emergency medicine, acute care and emergency nursing, experimental psychology, and psychometrics to assess dimensions of dyspnea intensity, sensory quality, unpleasantness, and dyspnea-related affective distress. The instrument’s structure and content are based on a theoretical model of dyspnea sensation and affect [29] that was derived from an extensively validated multidimensional model of pain [3036] that proposes potentially discriminable dimensions of sensation (intensity and quality) and two affective stages: immediate unpleasantness and emotional distress (e.g., judgments as to the meaning or significance of the experience). The relevance of this model to dyspnea is supported by multiple lines of laboratory and clinical research in dyspnea that have demonstrated the potential separability of dyspnea intensity and its associated emotional distress [3744] or unpleasantness [26, 45, 46] as well as mechanistic distinctions among dyspnea sensory qualities (e.g., different peripheral afferent pathways or higher central nervous system processing) [19, 25, 42, 4758].

The MDP has a total of 12 items that use 0-to-10 numerical rating scales. Single items are used to rate the overall intensity of breathing sensation (0 = No sensation; 10 = Maximum sensation) and its unpleasantness (0 = Neutral; 10 = Unbearable). Five items measure the intensity of groupings of potentially distinguishable sensory qualities (0 = None; 10 = As intense as I can imagine):

· My breathing requires muscle work or effort.

· I am not getting enough air, I feel hunger for air, or I am smothering.

· My breathing requires mental effort or concentration.

· My chest and lungs feel tight or constricted.

· I am breathing a lot (breathing rapidly, deeply, or heavily).

(In the initial protocol, there was a single descriptor for Work or Effort; after enrollment of 27 patients, the MDP was amended to better distinguish between muscle work or effort and mental effort or concentration.) Five items measure emotions in relation to “how your breathing sensations make you feel”: Depressed, Anxious, Frustrated, Angry, and Afraid (0 = None; 10 = The most I can imagine).

We administered the MDP several times over the course of the ED visit, with questions referring to how breathing felt at that particular time (“now” wording) or how breathing felt at the time the participant decided to come to the ED (“recall” wording). Apart from the difference in time frame, the instructions and questions were identical.

Support for the potential independence of MDP ratings of intensity from unpleasantness and work/effort from air hunger have been reported in controlled physiological experiments in a laboratory setting [26]. However, principal components analysis of “now” ratings using the MDP in ED patients showed two components (domains) that jointly accounted for 66% to 74% of item variance [28]. The first domain comprised the single-item ratings of intensity and unpleasantness together with the five sensory quality ratings and was labeled Immediate Perception (7 items; Cronbach’s α > .90). The second domain consisted of the ratings of breathing-related emotional distress and was labeled Emotional Response (5 items; Cronbach’s α ≥ .84).

Protocol

ED phase

Patients were triaged according to established departmental procedures. The initial contact for study participation took place after they had been evaluated and treatment was under way. Potentially eligible participants were identified by study staff, and the visit record was screened for excluding conditions. After ascertaining from the physician or registered nurse staff that the patient was sufficiently stable to be approached, potential participants were informed by ED personnel that a study was ongoing for which they might be eligible and given a brochure about the study prior to the initial contact by study staff. After the initial contact, those who expressed interest in participating were given a copy of the consent form and given time to read and consider it. After answering any questions, signed consent and full HIPAA authorization forms were obtained from all who agreed to participate.

As soon as possible after enrollment (Time 1), the study questionnaire was administered to assess how breathing felt at that time (“now” wording) and in a separate administration that asked participants to recall and rate how their breathing felt when they decided to come to the ED (“recall” wording: Time 0).

In the initial protocol, there was only a single administration of the Time 0 questionnaire (i.e., using the recall wording), but there were two subsequent administrations of the questionnaire using the “now” wording: an hour after the initial administration (Time 2) and, if possible, a third administration prior to leaving the department (Time 3). After 27 participants had been enrolled, a protocol amendment added a second recall administration immediately following the Time 2 administration of the “now” questionnaire. For the remainder of this report, the two recall administrations in the ED are referred to as Time 0a (at enrollment) and Time 0b (approximately an hour later). In general, the first questionnaire administration took no more than 5 minutes; subsequent administrations generally took less time.

Follow-up phase

As part of the consent process in the ED, potentially eligible persons were asked to indicate on the consent form whether they were willing to be contacted by study personnel at a later date to inquire about whether they might be willing to participate in a follow-up visit 4 to 6 weeks after the ED visit. Participation in the ED phase of the study was not conditional on whether or not they were willing to be contacted. Those who gave permission to be contacted for follow-up were invited to schedule an appointment. Participants with mobility or transportation issues were permitted to arrange a home visit if that was more convenient for them. The follow-up visit required a separate consent. The median (25th, 75th percentile) time to the follow-up visit was 5 (4, 7) weeks. During the follow-up visit, participants completed several questionnaires, including a third recall administration of the MDP (Time 0c) to reassess how their breathing felt when they decided to come to the ED.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using IBM® SPSS® Statistics, version 19. Descriptive statistics included mean and standard deviation or median and percentiles for continuous variables and counts and percentages for categorical variables.

Principal components analysis with varimax rotation was used to assess the similarity of domains for the recall ratings to those previously reported for “now” ratings in the ED [28] (see Additional file 1 for details). Cronbach’s alpha was assessed for each domain at Times 0a, 0b, and 0c. A mean score (total of item scores/# of items) was calculated for each domain to standardize the domain score to the same 0-to-10 metric as the constituent items. Test–retest reliability of the recall ratings was assessed using two-way mixed-model ICCs for absolute agreement at the level of individual items (single measures ICC) and mean domain scores (average measures ICC).

Mean paired differences and 95% CIs for recall ratings were assessed graphically for individual items and domains across measurement intervals (Time 0a–Time 0b and Time 0a–Time 0c). Because item and domain scores were not normally distributed, Wilcoxon signed rank tests were calculated between Time 0a and 0b and between Time 0a and 0c for all items and the two domain scores. In addition, within-subjects differences between Times 0a–0b and 0a–0c were estimated at the 5th, 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, 90th, and 95th percentiles, and Hodges–Lehmann (nonparametric) estimates of median difference [59] with 95% CIs were calculated.

Results

The sample consisted of 154 participants who were enrolled after the protocol amendment and for whom complete data were available on at least the Time 0a questionnaire. There were no significant differences in sex, age, race, or ethnicity between those who enrolled before versus after the protocol amendment.

The mean (SD) age of the sample was 53.2 (15.7) years; 45% (n = 70) were female; 78% were white, 7% were black, 5% were American Indian, and 10% were “other” or more than one race. Twenty-five per cent were Hispanic. Approximately 26% of participants (n = 41) had a diagnosis of COPD, 28% (n = 43) had asthma, 10% (n = 16) had heart failure, 16% (n = 25) had pneumonia, and 19% (n =29) had other cardiopulmonary diagnoses.

The component structure and domains for the MDP recall ratings were the same as reported previously [28] for the “now” ratings in these ED patients. For the three recall administrations, the Immediate Perception domain (7 items; Cronbach’s α = .89 to .94) and Emotional Response domain (5 items: Cronbach’s α = .81 to .85) jointly accounted for 63% to 71% of item variance (see Additional file 1: Table A1 — Principal components analysis).

Means, SDs, and quartiles for the MDP items and the two mean domain scores for each recall time period are shown in Table 1. Means for the Immediate Perception items were consistently higher than for the Emotional Response items in all three recall assessments (Table 1). The mean domain scores were approximately 2 scale points higher for the Immediate Perception domain compared with the Emotional Response domain in each recall assessment (Table 2). The Time 0a recall ratings and the concurrently obtained Time 1 “now” ratings were moderately and positively correlated for all items (Immediate Perception items: r = .30 to .45, p < .001; Emotional Response items: r = .46 to .60, p < .001) and domain scores (r = .42, p < .001 for Immediate Perception; r = .61, p < .001 for Emotional Response).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for recall rating: how breathing felt “when you decided to come to the ED”
Table 2 Within-subjects differences in mean scores for Immediate Perception vs. Emotional Response domains

Test–retest ICCs between recall ratings for the approximate 1-hour interval between Times 0a and 0b (Table 3) ranged from .69 to .86 for the individual items and .92 to .94 for the two domains. For the 4- to 6-week interval between the initial recall rating and the follow-up visit, ICCs were much lower (.28 to .66 for individual items and.72 to .78 for the two domains). Examination of 95% CIs around ICCs for the two test–retest intervals showed no overlap; therefore, all ICCs were significantly lower for the longer test–retest interval.

Table 3 Test–retest reliability of MDP recall ratings (single items and domains)

For the test–retest interval during the ED visit (Time 0a to 0b; Figure 1), mean differences for individual items ranged from −0.02 to +0.57 points, with all but two falling between 0.1 and 0.5 points. The mean differences for the two mean domain scores were approximately +0.3 points for Immediate Perception and EmotionalResponse. All but one of the mean differences were positive, indicating that the second set of recall ratings in the ED (Time 0b) was consistently lower (less severe) than the initial ratings (Time 0a).

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean within-subjects differences (95% CI) Time 0a–0b (during ED visit) for individual items and subscales for Immediate Perception and Emotional Response ( n= 141–145). Time 0a: time of decision to come to ED recalled at enrollment during ED visit. Time 0b: time of decision to come to ED recalled 1 hr after enrollment during ED visit.

For the test–retest interval between the ED visit and follow-up visit (Time 0a to 0c; Figure 2) mean differences for individual items ranged from −0.55 to +0.33 points, with all but 2 falling between −0.1 and −0.5 points. The mean differences for the two mean domain scores were approximately −0.2 points for Immediate Perception and −0.5 points for Emotional Response. All but one of the mean differences were negative, indicating that the follow-up ratings 4 to 6 weeks later were consistently higher (more severe) at Time 0c (during follow-up) than the initial ratings in the ED (Time 0a). However, the 95% CIs for the Time 0a–Time 0c differences (Figure 2) all contained 0 difference and were much wider than the 95% CIs for Time 0a–Time 0b differences (Figure 1).

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean within-subjects differences (95% CI) Time 0a–0c (ED to follow-up) for individual items and subscales for Immediate Perception and Emotional Response ( n= 67–68). Time 0a: time of decision to come to ED recalled at enrollment during ED visit. Time 0c: time of decision to come to ED recalled at follow-up visit 4–6 weeks after ED visit.

Percentiles of agreement were narrower (i.e., agreement was closer) for the Immediate Perception items compared with the Emotional Response items for both recall intervals (see Additional file 1: Table A2 — Percentiles of within-subjects differences). For the Time 0a to 0b interval (during the ED visit), 80% of subjects (10th, 90th percentiles) had scores within ± 1 point for the mean Immediate Perception domain score and within ± 2 points for the mean Emotional Response domain score. The ranges between percentiles of agreement were considerably wider for the Time 0a to 0c interval.

Discussion

Overall, internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) was strong for both MDP domains (Immediate Perception and Emotional Response) across all three recall administrations. During the ED visit, test–retest reliability in recall MDP ratings for dyspnea at the time participants decided to seek care in the ED was strong for individual items and very strong for the two domains (Table 3). Within-subjects agreement (intra-rater reliability) was satisfactory for both domains (Additional file 1: Table A2). In contrast, for the much longer recall interval between the ED and follow-up visits, the test–retest reliability (Table 3) and within-subjects agreement (Additional file 1: Table A2) were poor for individual items and significantly attenuated for the two domains.

For the short recall interval during the ED visit, the median within-subjects difference in scores was 0 for individual items and from 0 to 0.2 for the mean domain scores (Additional file 1: Table A2). There was a small but consistent shift toward lower ratings on the second administration in the ED (Table 1). Assuming the earliest recall rating as the reference standard, the consistency and amount of shift indicates a systematic error or bias of approximately +0.3 points on average as reflected in the positive mean within-subjects differences (Figure 1). This shift was also evident in absolute values of within-subjects differences at the 75th, 90th, and 95th percentiles generally exceeding the corresponding absolute values at the 25th, 10th, and 5th percentiles, respectively (Additional file 1: Table A2).

For the much longer test–retest interval between the ED and follow-up visits, median within-subjects differences were 0 for individual items and −0.2 to +0.1 for the mean domain scores (Additional file 1: Table A2). There was a small but consistent shift toward higher recall ratings at the follow-up compared with the initial recall ratings in the ED. This was reflected in the negative mean within-subjects differences of approximately −0.3 points for the Immediate Perception items and −0.5 points for the Emotional Response items (Figure 2). This shift was also evident in absolute values of within-subjects differences at the 25th, 10th, and 5th percentiles generally exceeding the absolute values of differences at the 75th, 90th, and 95th percentiles (Additional file 1: Table A2).

The magnitude of these shifts was small across both test–retest intervals. In addition, the 95% CI for differences for a majority of the individual items in Figure 1 (Time 0a–Time 0b) and for all individual items and domain scores in Figure 2 (Time 0a–Time 0c) are consistent with 0 difference, and the 95% CI in Figure 2 are much wider than in Figure 1. However, within each recall interval, the shifts were in same direction throughout the percentile distributions of within-subjects differences for items and domains (Additional file 1: Table A2), suggesting that the shifts are not due to outliers. In Figure 1, it is noteworthy that the point estimates for mean paired differences are > 0 for each mean domain score and for 11 of 12 individual items, whereas in Figure 2, the point estimates for mean paired differences are < 0 for each mean domain score and for 11 of 12 individual items. The consistency of those shifts within each test–retest interval is unlikely under a null hypothesis of random error around 0 difference and, on that basis, we believe systematic error (bias) to be a more plausible explanation. However, these shifts were not anticipated findings and deserve further investigation before any firm conclusions can be drawn.

We found that test–retest reliability for the items and mean domain score for Immediate Perception was stronger than for the Emotional Response items and domain score. In several studies in the pain literature, recall was more reliable and accurate for sensory compared with affective ratings [60] or pain descriptor choices [61].Although the component structure of the MDP recall ratings was similar across administrations, one notable difference was that Frustrated was the Emotional Response item with the strongest loading in both ED administrations, whereas Afraid was the strongest loading Emotional Response item during the follow-up visits (Additional file 1: Table A1).

In contrast to our findings, studies of neurological symptoms, specifically dizziness [3] and headache [4], have found substantial imprecision or lack of concordance in response to the same questions on two occasions in the ED [3] or to two semantically similar questions asked concurrently [4]. However, in both of those studies, the recall or concordance task involved nominal categories (i.e., qualitative descriptor categories [3] or dichotomous, yes/no type, choices [4]), not rating scales (as in the present study). It may well be the case for self-reported symptoms that test–retest reliability (or the assessment thereof) is facilitated if numerical rating scales are used rather than nominal (unordered) categorical choices. Alternatively, it is conceivable that symptom recall in the ED may be more reliable for dyspnea than it is for dizziness or headache.

An important limitation of the study is that we were unable to measure pre-arrival dyspnea in real time. The use of recall ratings was necessitated by limitations on approaching patients for participation until after initial clinical evaluation. In addition, the protocol did not include objective measures related to dyspnea during the ED visit against which the recall ratings could be assessed. However, in a previous publication [28] MDP “now” ratings during the follow-up visit were significantly and positively correlated with other measures of functional limitation due to breathlessness or fatigue, somatization, depression, and anxiety.

Other study limitations included convenience sampling, exclusion of patients who were unstable, and practical and ethical constraints on when initial contacts with patients and enrollment could occur relative to arrival in the ED. In addition, there were several limitations to our statistical analysis. Convenience sampling is difficult to avoid in observational studies with acutely ill patients, and we necessarily had to exclude patients who were unstable or whose capacity to consent was adversely impacted by their condition. Although participation was limited to English-speaking patients, nearly all exclusions on that basis were of patients who were Spanish speaking. Nonetheless, more than a quarter of participants were Hispanic. With respect to statistical analysis, we used principal components analysis rather than factor analysis to assess domain structure of the recall ratings. Estimates for component loadings, communalities, and total explained variance tend to be somewhat inflated for principal components compared with factor analysis. However, they generally agree on the number of components or factors to keep and which items load primarily on which factors [6264] (see Additional file 1: Principal components analysis and Table A1).

At the same time, several strengths of this study are notable. Apart from the limitations noted above, our inclusion criteria were broad, and our sample was diagnostically heterogeneous, suggesting that use of the MDP in the ED is not diagnosis-specific. We believe that enhances its potential usefulness in the ED. In conjunction with previous evidence of internal validity of the MDP (e.g., that items can discriminate between different dyspnea stimuli in controlled experiments [26] and that “now” ratings are responsive to clinical change in the ED [28]), results of the present study support its external validity. In addition, as recommended by Broderick and colleagues [5], we used a multiple-item instrument, gave clear and consistent instructions as to the rating task and dimensions to be rated, and recall was referenced to a specific point in time, the decision to come to the ED. Our results demonstrate high reliability in dyspnea recall when using the MDP during an ED visit and a high degree of similarity in factorial structure to MDP “now” ratings obtained after initiation of treatment [28]. However, we also found that test–retest reliability was poor for individual items and markedly decreased for domain scores over a 4- to 6-week recall interval between the ED and follow-up visits.

Conclusion

At a fundamental level, reliability estimates can be thought of as signal-to-noise ratios [18]. Undoubtedly, there is greater noise in symptom self-reports than in many measures of more objective data. However, at least some of the noise in symptom self-reports comes from asking noisy (e.g., ambiguous or poorly focused) questions, a problem that is potentially treatable by using a reliable and valid questionnaire such as the MDP [2628]. Although it might seem intuitive that one should ask patients to recall pre-visit events or perceptions as soon as possible after arrival in the ED, the results of this study suggest that within the span of an ED visit, recall of dyspnea is sufficiently stable that the actual time lag between arrival and a more detailed assessment with the MDP may not be critical while the patient is in the ED and should not be viewed as a barrier to the use of this measure in the ED.

Authors’ information

MBP is Associate Professor, University of New Mexico College of Nursing. PMM is Professor, University of Colorado Denver, College of Nursing. DS is Professor, Department of Emergency Medicine and Associate Dean for Graduate Medical Education, University of New Mexico School of Medicine. JA is Associate Professor, Department of Emergency Medicine, University of New Mexico School of Medicine and Chief of the Emergency Medicine Service, Raymond G. Murphy VA Medical Center, Albuquerque, NM. PB is retired. At the time the study was conducted, she was Project Manager, University of New Mexico, College of Nursing.

References

  1. Gallagher EJ, Bijur PE, Latimer C, Silver W: Reliability and validity of a visual analog scale for acute abdominal pain in the ED. Am J Emerg Med. 2002, 20 (4): 287-290. 10.1053/ajem.2002.33778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Bailey B, Daoust R, Doyon-Trottier E, Dauphin-Pierre S, Gravel J: Validation and properties of the verbal numeric scale in children with acute pain. Pain. 2010, 149 (2): 216-221. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Newman-Toker DE, Cannon LM, Stofferahn ME, Rothman RE, Hsieh Y-H, Zee DS: Imprecision in patient reports of dizziness symptom quality: a cross-sectional study conducted in an acute care setting. Mayo Clin Proc. 2007, 82 (11): 1329-1340. 10.4065/82.11.1329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Diaz M, Braude D, Skipper B: Concordance of historical questions used in risk-stratifying patients with headache. Am J Emerg Med. 2007, 25 (8): 907-910. 10.1016/j.ajem.2007.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Broderick JE, Stone AA, Calvanese P, Schwartz JE, Turk DC: Recalled pain ratings: a complex and poorly defined task. J Pain. 2006, 7 (2): 142-149. 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.09.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Meek PM, Lareau SC, Anderson D: Memory for symptoms in COPD patients: how accurate are their reports?. Eur Respir J. 2001, 18 (3): 474-481. 10.1183/09031936.01.00083501.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. American Thoracic Society: Dyspnea. Mechanisms, assessment, and management: a consensus statement. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1999, 159 (1): 321-340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Parshall MB, Schwartzstein RM, Adams L, Banzett RB, Manning HL, Bourbeau J, Calverley PM, Gift AG, Harver A, Lareau SC, et al: An Official ATS Statement: update on the mechanisms, assessment, and management of dyspnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2012, 185 (4): 435-452. 10.1164/rccm.201111-2042ST.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Aaron SD, Vandemheen KL, Clinch JJ, Ahuja J, Brison RJ, Dickinson G, Hébert PC: Measurement of short-term changes in dyspnea and disease-specific quality of life following an acute COPD exacerbation. Chest. 2002, 121 (3): 688-696. 10.1378/chest.121.3.688.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Tsai C-L, Hodder RV, Page JH, Cydulka RK, Rowe BH, Camargo CA: The short-form Chronic Respiratory Disease Questionnaire was a valid, reliable, and responsive quality-of-life instrument in acute exacerbations of chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. J Clin Epidemiol. 2008, 61 (5): 489-497. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Kendrick KR, Baxi SC, Smith RM: Usefulness of the modified 0–10 Borg scale in assessing the degree of dyspnea in patients with COPD and asthma. J Emerg Nurs. 2000, 26 (3): 216-222.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Saracino A: Validation of a verbal dyspnoea rating scale in the emergency department. Emerg Med Australas. 2008, 20 (6): 475-481. 10.1111/j.1742-6723.2008.01132.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Mebazaa A, Pang PS, Tavares M, Collins SP, Storrow AB, Laribi S, Andre S, Mark Courtney D, Hasa J, Spinar J, et al: The impact of early standard therapy on dyspnoea in patients with acute heart failure: the URGENT-dyspnoea study. Eur Heart J. 2010, 31 (7): 832-841. 10.1093/eurheartj/ehp458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Saracino A, Weiland TJ, Jolly B, Dent AW: Verbal dyspnoea score predicts emergency department departure status in patients with shortness of breath. Emerg Med Australas. 2010, 22 (1): 21-29. 10.1111/j.1742-6723.2009.01254.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Camargo CA, Tsai C-L, Clark S, Kenney PA, Radeos MS: Spirometric correlates of dyspnea improvement among emergency department patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease exacerbation. Respir Care. 2008, 53 (7): 892-896.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Clark WC, Yang JC, Tsui S-L, Ng K-F, Bennett Clark S: Unidimensional pain rating scales: a multidimensional affect and pain survey (MAPS) analysis of what they really measure. Pain. 2002, 98 (3): 241-247. 10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00474-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. DeVellis RF: Scale Development: Theory and Applications 3rd edition. 2012, Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  18. Streiner DL, Norman GR: Health Measurement Scales: A Practical Guide to Their Development and Use. 2008, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 4

    Book  Google Scholar 

  19. Parshall MB: Psychometric characteristics of dyspnea descriptor ratings in emergency department patients with exacerbated chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Res Nurs Health. 2002, 25 (5): 331-344. 10.1002/nur.10051.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Shrout PE, Fleiss JL: Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull. 1979, 86 (2): 420-428.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. McGraw KO, Wong SP: Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychol Methods. 1996, 1 (1): 30-46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Weir JP: Quantifying test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. J Strength Cond Res. 2005, 19 (1): 231-240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Parshall MB, Carle AC, Ice U, Taylor R, Powers J: Validation of a 3-factor measurement model of dyspnea in hospitalized adults with heart failure. Heart Lung. 2012, 41 (1): 44-56. 10.1016/j.hrtlng.2011.05.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Moy ML, Lantin ML, Harver A, Schwartzstein RM: Language of dyspnea in assessment of patients with acute asthma treated with nebulized albuterol. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1998, 158 (3): 749-753.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Ekman I, Granger B, Swedberg K, Stenlund H, Boman K: Measuring shortness of breath in heart failure (SOB-HF): development and validation of a new dyspnoea assessment tool. Eur J Heart Fail. 2011, 13 (8): 838-845. 10.1093/eurjhf/hfr062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Banzett RB, Pedersen SH, Schwartzstein RM, Lansing RW: The affective dimension of laboratory dyspnea: air hunger is more unpleasant than work/effort. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2008, 177 (12): 1384-1390. 10.1164/rccm.200711-1675OC.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Banzett RB, Adams L, O’Donnell CR, Gilman SA, Lansing RW, Schwartzstein RM: Using laboratory models to test treatment: Morphine reduces dyspnea and hypercapnic ventilatory response. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2011, 184 (8): 920-927. 10.1164/rccm.201101-0005OC.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Meek PM, Banzett RB, Parshall MB, Gracely RH, Schwartzstein RM, Lansing R: Reliability and validity of the multidimensional dyspnea profile (MDP). Chest. January 19, 2012

    Google Scholar 

  29. Lansing RW, Gracely RH, Banzett RB: The multiple dimensions of dyspnea: review and hypotheses. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2009, 167 (1): 53-60. 10.1016/j.resp.2008.07.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Wade JB, Price DD, Hamer RM, Schwartz SM, Hart RP: An emotional component analysis of chronic pain. Pain. 1990, 40 (3): 303-310. 10.1016/0304-3959(90)91127-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Gracely RH: Evaluation of multi-dimensional pain scales. Pain. 1992, 48 (3): 297-300. 10.1016/0304-3959(92)90076-N.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Gracely RH: Affective dimensions of pain: how many and how measured?. Am Pain Soc J. 1992, 1 (4): 243-247.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Price DD, Harkins SW: The affective-motivational dimension of pain: a two-stage model. Am Pain Soc J. 1992, 1 (4): 229-239.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Wade JB, Dougherty LM, Archer CR, Price DD: Assessing the stages of pain processing: a multivariate analytical approach. Pain. 1996, 68 (1): 157-167. 10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03162-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Price DD: Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science. 2000, 288 (5472): 1769-1772. 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Price DD: Central neural mechanisms that interrelate sensory and affective dimensions of pain. Mol Interv. 2002, 2 (6): 392-403. 10.1124/mi.2.6.392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wilson RC, Jones PW: Differentiation between the intensity of breathlessness and the distress it evokes in normal subjects during exercise. Clin Sci. 1991, 80 (1): 65-70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Carrieri-Kohlman V, Gormley JM, Douglas MK, Paul SM, Stulbarg MS: Differentiation between dyspnea and its affective components. West J Nurs Res. 1996, 18 (6): 626-642. 10.1177/019394599601800602.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Carrieri-Kohlman V, Gormley JM, Eiser S, Demir-Deviren S, Nguyen H, Paul SM, Stulbarg MS: Dyspnea and the affective response during exercise training in obstructive pulmonary disease. Nurs Res. 2001, 50 (3): 136-146. 10.1097/00006199-200105000-00002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Carrieri-Kohlman V, Donesky-Cuenco D, Park SK, Mackin L, Nguyen HQ, Paul SM: Additional evidence for the affective dimension of dyspnea in patients with COPD. Res Nurs Health. 2010, 23 (1): 4-19.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Swigris JJ, Yorke J, Sprunger DB, Swearingen C, Pincus T, du Bois RM, Brown KK, Fischer A: Assessing dyspnea and its impact on patients with connective tissue disease-related interstitial lung disease. Respir Med. 2010, 104 (9): 1350-1355. 10.1016/j.rmed.2010.03.027.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Yorke J, Moosavi SH, Shuldham C, Jones PW: Quantification of dyspnoea using descriptors: development and initial testing of the Dyspnoea-12. Thorax. 2010, 65 (1): 21-26. 10.1136/thx.2009.118521.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Yorke J, Russell A-M, Swigris J, Shuldham C, Haigh C, Rochnia N, Hoyle J, Jones PW: Assessment of dyspnea in asthma: validation of the Dyspnea-12. J Asthma. 2011, 48 (6): 602-608. 10.3109/02770903.2011.585412.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Yorke J, Swigris J, Russell A-M, Moosavi SH, Ng Man Kwong G, Longshaw M, Jones PW: Dyspnea-12 is a valid and reliable measure of breathlessness in patients with interstitial lung disease. Chest. 2011, 139 (1): 159-164. 10.1378/chest.10-0693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, Baumann HJ, Klose H, Dahme B, Buchel C: The unpleasantness of perceived dyspnea is processed in the anterior insula and amygdala. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2008, 177: 1026-1032. 10.1164/rccm.200712-1821OC.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Wan L, Van Diest I, De Peuter S, Bogaerts K, Van den Bergh O: Repeated breathlessness experiences induced by hypercapnia: differential effects on intensity and unpleasantness. Chest. 2009, 135 (2): 455-461. 10.1378/chest.08-1226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Simon PM, Schwartzstein RM, Weiss JW, Lahive K, Fencl V, Teghtsoonian M, Weinberger SE: Distinguishable sensations of breathlessness induced in normal volunteers. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1989, 140 (4): 1021-1027.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Simon PM, Schwartzstein RM, Weiss JW, Fencl V, Teghtsoonian M, Weinberger SE: Distinguishable types of dyspnea in patients with shortness of breath. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1990, 142 (5): 1009-1014.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Elliott MW, Adams L, Cockcroft A, MacRae KD, Murphy K, Guz A: The language of breathlessness. Use of verbal descriptors by patients with cardiopulmonary disease. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1991, 144 (4): 826-832.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Mahler DA, Harver A, Lentine T, Scott JA, Beck K, Schwartzstein RM: Descriptors of breathlessness in cardiorespiratory diseases. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1996, 154 (5): 1357-1363.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hardie GE, Janson S, Gold WM, Carrieri-Kohlman V, Boushey HA: Ethnic differences: word descriptors used by African-American and white asthma patients during induced bronchoconstriction. Chest. 2000, 117 (4): 935-943. 10.1378/chest.117.4.935.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Harver A, Mahler DA, Schwartzstein RM, Baird JC: Descriptors of breathlessness in healthy individuals: distinct and separable constructs. Chest. 2000, 118 (3): 679-690. 10.1378/chest.118.3.679.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Parshall MB, Welsh JD, Brockopp DY, Heiser RM, Schooler MP, Cassidy KB: Reliability and validity of dyspnea sensory quality descriptors in heart failure patients treated in an emergency department. Heart Lung. 2001, 30 (1): 57-65. 10.1067/mhl.2001.112499.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Wilcock A, Crosby V, Hughes A, Fielding K, Corcoran R, Tattersfield AE: Descriptors of breathlessness in patients with cancer and other cardiorespiratory diseases. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2002, 23 (3): 182-189. 10.1016/S0885-3924(01)00417-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Williams M, Garrard A, Cafarella P, Petkov J, Frith P: Quality of recalled dyspnoea is different from exercise-induced dyspnoea: an experimental study. Aust J Physiother. 2009, 55 (3): 177-183. 10.1016/S0004-9514(09)70078-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Williams M, Cafarella P, Olds T, Petkov J, Frith P: Affective descriptors of the sensation of breathlessness are more highly associated with severity of impairment than physical descriptors in people with COPD. Chest. 2010, 138 (2): 315-322. 10.1378/chest.09-2498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Bianchi R, Gigliotti F, Romagnoli I, Lanini B, Castellani C, Binazzi B, Stendardi L, Bruni GI, Scano G: Impact of a rehabilitation program on dyspnea intensity and quality in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Respiration. 2011, 81 (3): 186-195. 10.1159/000273675.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Harver A, Schwartzstein RM, Kotses H, Humphries CT, Schmaling KB, Mullin ML: Descriptors of breathlessness in children with persistent asthma. Chest. 2011, 139 (4): 832-838. 10.1378/chest.10-2388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Hodges JL, Lehmann EL: Estimates of location based on ranks. Ann Math Stat. 1963, 34: 598-611. 10.1214/aoms/1177704172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Hunter M, Philips C, Rachman S: Memory for pain. Pain. 1979, 6 (1): 35-46. 10.1016/0304-3959(79)90138-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Beese A, Morley S: Memory for acute pain experience is specifically inaccurate but generally reliable. Pain. 1993, 53 (2): 183-189. 10.1016/0304-3959(93)90079-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Norman GR, Streiner DL: Biostatistics: The Bare Essentials. 2007, People’s Medical Publishing House, Shelton, CT

    Google Scholar 

  63. Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH: Psychometric Theory. 1994, McGraw-Hill, New York, 3

    Google Scholar 

  64. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS: Using Multivariate Statistics. 2007, Pearson, Boston, 5

    Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health [Grant NR010006; PI Robert B. Banzett] and by the New Mexico VA Health Care System, Albuquerque, New Mexico. We wish to express our heartfelt thanks to our participants and to the physicians and nurses of the Emergency Departments of the University of New Mexico Hospital, the Raymond G. Murphy VA Medical Center, and Presbyterian Kaseman Hospital. In particular, we wish to thank Michael Richards, MD, Cameron Crandall, MD, and Michael Chicarelli, RN, MSN, of University of New Mexico; Henry Holmes, RN, Raymond G. Murphy VA Medical Center and Annie Cook, Sandra Diesel, Gaylene Vargas, and Barbara Gabaldon of the NMVAHCS Research Service; and Johanna Stiesmeyer, RN, MSN, and Larraine Yeager, RN, MSN, of Presbyterian Healthcare Services, Albuquerque NM, for their support. We are deeply grateful to Robert Banzett, PhD, Richard Schwartzstein, MD, and Robert Lansing, PhD, of Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Harvard School of Medicine, for their collaboration in the development of the MDP and for their support, encouragement, and critical discussion. We also wish to thank: Nancy Ridenour, PhD, RN, FAAN, Dean of the University of New Mexico College of Nursing, and Patricia Moritz, PhD, RN, FAAN, Dean of the University of Colorado College of Nursing, for their support; Ms. Anne Mattarella of the University of New Mexico College of Nursing for expert assistance with technical editing; and the reviewers of the manuscript for their helpful suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mark B Parshall.

Additional information

Competing interests

MBP, PMM, DS, JA, and PB have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MBP and PMM conceived of the study and participated in all aspects of its design and coordination, and planned and conducted the statistical analysis. DS and JA participated in the design of the study protocol, data acquisition, and interpretation of results. PB participated in data acquisition and study coordination. MBP wrote the initial draft, and all authors participated in revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12873_2011_139_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: Table A1. Principal components analysis with varimax rotation for MDP recall ratings. Table A2 Percentiles of within-subjects differences. (DOC 122 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Parshall, M.B., Meek, P.M., Sklar, D. et al. Test–retest reliability of multidimensional dyspnea profile recall ratings in the emergency department: a prospective, longitudinal study. BMC Emerg Med 12, 6 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-227X-12-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-227X-12-6

Keywords